Graham Welch: Everyone is musical, given the opportunity

From a lecture given at the University of London's Institute of Education by the Professor of Music Education

Monday 28 January 2002 20:00 EST
Comments

Your support helps us to tell the story

From reproductive rights to climate change to Big Tech, The Independent is on the ground when the story is developing. Whether it's investigating the financials of Elon Musk's pro-Trump PAC or producing our latest documentary, 'The A Word', which shines a light on the American women fighting for reproductive rights, we know how important it is to parse out the facts from the messaging.

At such a critical moment in US history, we need reporters on the ground. Your donation allows us to keep sending journalists to speak to both sides of the story.

The Independent is trusted by Americans across the entire political spectrum. And unlike many other quality news outlets, we choose not to lock Americans out of our reporting and analysis with paywalls. We believe quality journalism should be available to everyone, paid for by those who can afford it.

Your support makes all the difference.

I was driving through London's rush hour one morning listening to a BBC interview with a member of a team who reporting on a study of pitch perception in the journal Science. The authors' conclusion, based on experimental data from identical female twins, was that "heritability for tune deafness is 0.80", or that 80 per cent of the observed variability in pitch perception among twins was genetic in origin.

He claimed that "these results demonstrate for the first time the powerful influence of genes on the ability of humans to recognise correct pitches and melodies." The BBC interviewer asked about the implications of these findings for parents whose children were learning to play musical instruments. The research scientist suggested that many parents were wasting their money.

Such a misunderstanding of the nature of musical behaviour is part of the folklore of music, namely that people are either "musical" or "unmusical". The neuropsychobiological research evidence indicates that everyone is musical (assuming normal anatomy and physiology).

The term "tone deaf" should be confined to that rare number of people who have some form of neurological disorder in the processing of sound. It is inappropriate, even as a conventional shorthand, for use with developing singers, because it ignores the reality, namely that "out-of-tune" singing behaviour should be understood commonly as a mismatch between the designated musical task and current singing competency. Adults (and children) who have self-labelled themselves as "tone-deaf" have been shown to improve and develop enhanced singing skills in an appropriate educational setting.

In an educational context, every effort should be made to counter the deficit model that has been explicitly nurtured through the use of negative labelling (such as "growlers" and "monotone") for developing singers. The following strategy outlined by W Carrol in The Training of Children's Voices (1922) is not recommended: "If there are 'growlers' in the class they should be at the side – about two yards away from the other scholars. These children should be allowed to join in occasionally, very quietly. They will often gain more from listening than from 'singing'."

Another misunderstanding is to assume that perceived singing competency is a reflection of a general level of musical competence. Singing is one form of musical behaviour. Others, such as composing, are subject to their own multi-faceted developmental processes related to particular experiences and contexts.

A final misconception concerns the word music itself. This suggests a single, perhaps unitary phenomenon. However, although such a generic conceptualisation can be useful, the reality is that there are many and diverse musics in our world. Within each society and its constituent communities, many musics flourish, not least as adjuncts to group and personal identities. Music education has to find a way to celebrate, understand and promote access to this diversity if it is to realise the musical potential of the individuals it is meant to serve.

In 1837, a social and moral experiment began of introducing vocal music into the curriculum of the schools of Boston. In May of the following year, the Mayor of Boston requested a report on progress. The school's response was subsequently published by the Boston Music Gazette on Wednesday, 25 July 1838:

"One thing has been made evident, that the musical ear is more common than has been generally supposed. Many who at the outset of the experiment believed that they had neither ear nor voice, now sing with confidence and with considerable accuracy; and others who could hardly tell one sound from another, now sing the scale with ease."

We are all musical; we just need the opportunity.

Join our commenting forum

Join thought-provoking conversations, follow other Independent readers and see their replies

Comments

Thank you for registering

Please refresh the page or navigate to another page on the site to be automatically logged inPlease refresh your browser to be logged in