Humans are ‘surprisingly’ poor at picking up cues of aggression in dogs, study says
Humans may be biased to assume good intentions from dogs and other humans
Your support helps us to tell the story
From reproductive rights to climate change to Big Tech, The Independent is on the ground when the story is developing. Whether it's investigating the financials of Elon Musk's pro-Trump PAC or producing our latest documentary, 'The A Word', which shines a light on the American women fighting for reproductive rights, we know how important it is to parse out the facts from the messaging.
At such a critical moment in US history, we need reporters on the ground. Your donation allows us to keep sending journalists to speak to both sides of the story.
The Independent is trusted by Americans across the entire political spectrum. And unlike many other quality news outlets, we choose not to lock Americans out of our reporting and analysis with paywalls. We believe quality journalism should be available to everyone, paid for by those who can afford it.
Your support makes all the difference.Humans “surprisingly” struggle to identify aggressive behavior in dogs and in other people, according to a new study that may help reduce dog-biting incidents.
Humans constantly interpret cues in social situations and the ability to tell whether another person or animal is happy or aggressive has major evolutionary advantages, said scientists from the Max Planck Institute in Germany.
The new study, published on Wednesday in the journal PLOS ONE, assessed how well people can evaluate social interactions and found that humans performed no better than chance at assessing aggression.
In the research, 92 adult participants viewed a series of short video clips showing the start of a non-verbal interaction between two children, two domestic dogs or two Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus).
The videos included signals about the nature of the interaction, such as body postures and facial expressions.
However, the clips were stopped just before the interaction took place.
Scientists asked half the participants to categorise the interaction as aggressive, neutral or playful, and the other half to predict the outcome from three possible options.
While the participants performed better than expected by chance at both tasks, they did not do so while assessing aggressive interactions in dogs and humans, researchers explained.
The participants were accurate in categorising playful interactions, which they correctly identified 70 per cent of the time, according to the study.
But they performed “particularly poorly” at predicting the outcome of aggressive interactions in dogs, researchers noted.
Scientists also found that people who were good at predicting outcomes for one species also performed better than average for the other species.
Humans may be biased to assume good intentions from other humans and dogs, something that may prevent the accurate recognition of aggresive interactions, researchers said.
To reduce dog biting incidents, new dog owners could benefit from improved education about dog behaviour and learning to identify aggressive interactions.
“Humans are quite good at categorising and predicting social situations with other humans, dogs and monkeys, but it depends on the context. Surprisingly, humans underestimate aggression in dogs,” scientists wrote in the study.
Join our commenting forum
Join thought-provoking conversations, follow other Independent readers and see their replies
Comments