Stay up to date with notifications from The Independent

Notifications can be managed in browser preferences.

Bumblebees ‘prioritise getting maximum calories in shortest time’

According to research, the insects trade off the time they spend collecting nectar with the energy content of that nectar.

Nina Massey
Tuesday 24 October 2023 11:00 EDT
Bumblebees prioritise getting maximum calories in the shortest time, research has found (Jacob King/PA)
Bumblebees prioritise getting maximum calories in the shortest time, research has found (Jacob King/PA) (PA Archive)

Your support helps us to tell the story

From reproductive rights to climate change to Big Tech, The Independent is on the ground when the story is developing. Whether it's investigating the financials of Elon Musk's pro-Trump PAC or producing our latest documentary, 'The A Word', which shines a light on the American women fighting for reproductive rights, we know how important it is to parse out the facts from the messaging.

At such a critical moment in US history, we need reporters on the ground. Your donation allows us to keep sending journalists to speak to both sides of the story.

The Independent is trusted by Americans across the entire political spectrum. And unlike many other quality news outlets, we choose not to lock Americans out of our reporting and analysis with paywalls. We believe quality journalism should be available to everyone, paid for by those who can afford it.

Your support makes all the difference.

Bumblebees prioritise getting as many calories as possible in the shortest amount of time, new research suggests.

According to the findings, the insects trade off the time they spend collecting nectar with the energy content of that nectar.

The most common bumblebees in the UK, Bombus terrestris, will forage to collect nectar that is difficult to access – but only if the sugar content of that nectar makes it worth doing so.

This approach contrasts with honeybee foraging – honeybees make their decisions by optimising their individual energy expenditure for any nectar they collect, researchers say.

This more measured approach should prolong the honeybee’s working life.

As they forage, bumblebees are making decisions about which nectar sources will give the greatest immediate energetic return, rather than optimising the energy efficiency of their foraging

Dr Jonathan Pattrick, University of Cambridge

Dr Jonathan Pattrick, joint first author of the report, who started the research while in the University of Cambridge’s Department of Plant Sciences, said: “As they forage, bumblebees are making decisions about which nectar sources will give the greatest immediate energetic return, rather than optimising the energy efficiency of their foraging.”

Now based at the University of Oxford, he added: “Our results allow us to make predictions about the sorts of flowers the bumblebees are likely to visit, which could inform the choice of which flowers to plant in field margins to support these important pollinators.

“It’s also relevant to crop breeders who want to make varieties that are ‘better’ for bumblebees.”

Over six months 60,000 behavioural observations were made of the bumblebees, allowing researchers to estimate the energy they used while foraging.

Researchers watched each bumblebee in the study for up to eight hours a day, without taking a break.

The study used vertically and horizontally oriented artificial flowers, slippery surfaces that were difficult for the bumblebees to grip.

A custom computer program was used to measure the split-second timing as the bumblebees flew between the artificial flowers and foraged from them.

Researchers tracked how much energy the creatures spent flying, as well as how much they collected when drinking.

They also identified whether the bumblebees decided to spend extra time and energy collecting high-sugar nectar from slippery flowers, or take the easier option of collecting lower-sugar nectar from flowers they could land on.

It’s amazing that, even with a brain smaller than a sesame seed, bumblebees can make such complex decisions

Dr Hamish Symington, University of Cambridge

Dr Hamish Symington, from the University of Cambridge’s Department of Plant Sciences and joint first author of the report, said: “It’s amazing that, even with a brain smaller than a sesame seed, bumblebees can make such complex decisions.”

He added: “It’s clear that bumblebee foraging isn’t based on a simple idea that ‘the more sugar there is in nectar, the better’ – it’s much more subtle than that.

“And it highlights that there’s still so much to learn about insect behaviour.”

Individual bumblebees were given one of three tests.

In the first test, the nectar on both vertical and horizontal artificial flowers had the same amount of sugar.

The bumblebees made the obvious choice to forage from the horizontal flowers, rather than spend extra time and energy hovering at the vertical ones.

In the second test, the nectar on the vertical flowers was much more sugary than the nectar on the horizontal flowers.

Here the bumblebees chose to drink almost exclusively from the vertical flowers.

In the third test, the vertical flowers offered nectar that was only slightly more sugary than the horizontal flowers.

In this situation the bumblebees had to make a trade-off between the time and energy they spent foraging and the energy in the nectar they were drinking – and they switched to feeding from the horizontal flowers.

According to the findings, bumblebees can choose to spend additional time and energy foraging from hard-to-access nectar sources – but only if the reward is worth it.

Bumblebees offload nectar from flowers – by regurgitation – for use by others in the nest.

Unlike honeybees, they only store a small amount of nectar in the nest, so they need to make the most of every opportunity to forage.

– The research, funded by BBSRC, is published in iScience.

Thank you for registering

Please refresh the page or navigate to another page on the site to be automatically logged inPlease refresh your browser to be logged in