Stay up to date with notifications from The Independent

Notifications can be managed in browser preferences.

12 optical illusions that show how colour can trick the eye

Illusions work perceptual boundaries, changing a colour's appearance by changing a background

Ana Swanson
Wednesday 30 December 2015 10:52 EST
Comments
The notorious dress
The notorious dress (Swiked/Tumblr)

Your support helps us to tell the story

From reproductive rights to climate change to Big Tech, The Independent is on the ground when the story is developing. Whether it's investigating the financials of Elon Musk's pro-Trump PAC or producing our latest documentary, 'The A Word', which shines a light on the American women fighting for reproductive rights, we know how important it is to parse out the facts from the messaging.

At such a critical moment in US history, we need reporters on the ground. Your donation allows us to keep sending journalists to speak to both sides of the story.

The Independent is trusted by Americans across the entire political spectrum. And unlike many other quality news outlets, we choose not to lock Americans out of our reporting and analysis with paywalls. We believe quality journalism should be available to everyone, paid for by those who can afford it.

Your support makes all the difference.

The Internet erupted in an energetic debate earlier this year about whether an ugly dress was blue and black or white and gold, with celebrities from Anna Kendrick (white) to Taylor Swift (black) weighing in. (For the record, I’m with Taylor – never a bad camp to be in.)

It sounds inane, but the dress question was actually tricky: Some declared themselves firmly in the blue and black camp, only to have the dress appear white and gold when they looked back a few hours later.

Wired had the best explanation of the science behind the dress’s shifting colours. When your brain tries to figure out what colour something is, it essentially subtracts the lighting and background colours around it, or as the neuroscientist interviewed by Wired says, tries to “discount the chromatic bias of the daylight axis.” This is why you can identify an apple as red whether you see it at noon or at dusk.

The dress is on some kind of perceptual boundary, with a pretty even mix of blue, red and green. (Frankly, it’s just a terrible, washed out photo.) So for those who see it as white, your eyes may be subtracting the wrong background and lighting.

Changing a colour’s appearance by changing the background or lighting is one of the most common techniques in optical illusions. As the examples below show, colours can change dramatically against different backgrounds. (If you’ve ever held a sock up to something black to see whether it was black or navy, you understand the concept.)

For example, in this classic shadow illusion by Edward H. Adelson, A and B are the exact same shade of grey:

Here's a minimalist illustration by Wikipedia user Dodek. The gray bar across the center is actually one constant colour:

In this image from BrainDen, the surface colours of A and B are the same. To test it out, just use your finger to cover the middle of the drawing, where the two squares meet.

In this illusion by Barton L. Anderson and Jonathan Winawer, the black and white chess pieces are the same colour:

If you want a dog of a different colour, just set it against a different background (via BrainDen):

There are actually only three colours in this image -- white, red and green (sorry, colour blind people). Also via BrainDen.

The blue and yellow border around this image by Jochen Burghardt creates the illusion that it is pale yellow, instead of white:

Contrasting colours can even give you the illusion of motion, as in this trippy graphic by Paul Nasca:

The same principle is at work in this motion illusion by Japanes professor Akiyoshi Kitaoka called "Rotating Snakes." As you stare at the image, the circles will appear to turn.

If you stare at the center of this illusion by Jeremy Hinton, you will eventually see a revolving green circle. When the lilac disappears, the adaptation of rods and cones in the retina leaves a green afterimage.

In Pinna's illusory intertwining effect, shown here in an illustration by Jochen Burghardt, colours give the illusion that circles are intertwining (they are actually concentric).

But probably the best illusion on the subject of the dress is by Randall Munroe of Xkcd, who immortalized the debate in an optical illusion cartoon form.

Washington Post

Join our commenting forum

Join thought-provoking conversations, follow other Independent readers and see their replies

Comments

Thank you for registering

Please refresh the page or navigate to another page on the site to be automatically logged inPlease refresh your browser to be logged in