Stay up to date with notifications from The Independent

Notifications can be managed in browser preferences.

‘Good-looking’ pupils perform better in school, research finds

New study finds that other indicators of academic success pale in comparison to looks

Christopher Ingraham
Sunday 03 November 2019 09:23 EST
Comments
A total of 29,580 new postgraduate trainees were recruited this year in England, an increase of just 365 extra teachers
A total of 29,580 new postgraduate trainees were recruited this year in England, an increase of just 365 extra teachers (PA)

Your support helps us to tell the story

From reproductive rights to climate change to Big Tech, The Independent is on the ground when the story is developing. Whether it's investigating the financials of Elon Musk's pro-Trump PAC or producing our latest documentary, 'The A Word', which shines a light on the American women fighting for reproductive rights, we know how important it is to parse out the facts from the messaging.

At such a critical moment in US history, we need reporters on the ground. Your donation allows us to keep sending journalists to speak to both sides of the story.

The Independent is trusted by Americans across the entire political spectrum. And unlike many other quality news outlets, we choose not to lock Americans out of our reporting and analysis with paywalls. We believe quality journalism should be available to everyone, paid for by those who can afford it.

Your support makes all the difference.

A new study finds that good-looking children do better in school than their less striking peers.

The research, by Barnard College economist Daniel Hamermesh and colleagues, finds that people whose looks are “one standard deviation above average” attain nearly five more months of schooling than an “otherwise identical average-looking individual”.

The finding is surprising, they write, because prior research has shown that many of the factors of educational achievement on which policymakers tend to focus, such as teacher quality and program effectiveness, have a relatively small affect on student achievement – “often nearly zero”.

To explore how looks affect educational performance, Mr Hamermesh and his colleagues turned to separate data sets that track the academic achievements of children over several years: the US Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, which followed more than 1,300 children aged from 6 months to 15 years old; and the UK National Child Development Study, which tracked 17,000 Britons born during a single week in 1958.

Beauty may be in the eye of the beholder, but social scientists still need a way to measure it. For each child in the American study, looks were assessed by a panel of at least 10 undergraduates, who watched segments of video interviews with the children collected over the course of the survey. The raters assessed each one’s looks on a scale from 1 (not at all cute/very unattractive) to 5 (very cute/very attractive).

For the UK study, attractiveness was assessed by the children’s teachers, who assigned each child to one of four categories: attractive, unattractive, “abnormal feature” or “underfed or scruffy and dirty”.

When it came to looks, the children in both studies were well above average, with raters issuing scores that leaned towards the high end of their respective scales. Mr Hamermesh has noted in previous work that this tends to happen when people rate adults as well: there’s something of a universal bias towards rating people as attractive.

Ratings in hand, the researchers then analysed the relationship between a child’s looks and academic achievement, as measured via various standardised tests administered throughout the two studies. The studies revealed the same general pattern: better looking children performed better on tests of academic achievement, even when controlling for ethnicity, gender and parents’ education and income.

Economists who’ve studied academic performance have found that children who perform better on tests tend to get more schooling. A student who aces all their reading and maths tests is much more likely to go on to college than one who does not. Mr Hamermesh and his colleagues estimate that good-looking children’s heightened test performance relative to their average-looking peers works out to about 0.4 years, or five months, of additional schooling.

To determine why, Mr Hamermesh and his colleagues tested a number of theories. They found some evidence that teachers report better relationships with the more attractive students, which explains some of the achievement gap. They also found that youngsters rated as unattractive were somewhat more likely to report being bullied by their peers, with detrimental spillover on their academic performance.

Similarly, they found that there were fewer reports of behavioural problems at school among the highly rated children.

But the effect of looks on grades remained much larger than all three of those factors –​ teacher-student relationships, bullying and behavioural issues – combined. That means that the ultimate mechanism by which looks affect schooling remains a mystery and thus fertile ground for future research.

Nonetheless, the study offers a persuasive answer to one question: why has previous research shown that better-looking people earn more money than their average-looking peers? Based on this new information, Mr Hamermesh and his colleagues write that “20 to as much as 80 per cent of the economic returns to beauty arises from its prior indirect effects on educational attainment”.

The good-looking among us, in other words, may have the odds tilted in their favour from the get-go. And although there may indeed be “more to life than being really, really, really, ridiculously good-looking”, in the words of Derek Zoolander, the attractive do seem to be positioned to get the most out of life.

The Washington Post

Join our commenting forum

Join thought-provoking conversations, follow other Independent readers and see their replies

Comments

Thank you for registering

Please refresh the page or navigate to another page on the site to be automatically logged inPlease refresh your browser to be logged in